Lastly, there was no apparent change in the levels of GABAB2 rece

Lastly, there was no apparent change in the levels of GABAB2 receptor protein (Figure 7F), suggesting little METH-dependent degradation of receptor. Dephosphorylation of GABAB2-p-S783 has been previously shown to be regulated by protein phosphatase 2A (PP2A; Terunuma et al., 2010), raising the possibility that in vivo exposure to METH enhances the phosphatase activity in VTA GABA neurons. To address this, we examined the effect of acutely inhibiting PP1/PP2A phosphatases with okadaic

acid (OA; 100 nM). In saline-injected mice, check details there was no significant difference in the amplitude of IBaclofen with OA in the pipet, suggesting that basal activity of PP1/PP2A does not significantly regulate GABABR-GIRKs (Figures 7G–7J). In METH-injected mice, however, intracellular application of OA promoted recovery of the IBaclofen (Figures 7H and 7J). Note the slow time course of activation for IBaclofen in the presence of OA in METH-injected mice. This increase could reflect insertion of GABAB receptors and GIRK channels on the plasma membrane or restoration of

functional G protein coupling. For control, we examined the effect of PKC(19-36), a peptide inhibitor of PKC (Figure 7K). Unlike OA, the presence see more of PKC inhibitor in the pipet did not restore IBaclofen, similar to the effect of METH alone. Taken together, these findings suggest that in vivo exposure to METH triggers a phosphatase-dependent downregulation of GABABRs and GIRK channels from the plasma membrane of GABA neurons, which results in reduced GABABR-GIRK signaling and accumulation of GABAB receptor complexes in intracellular compartments. To investigate the functional consequence of reduced GABABR-GIRK currents these in GABA neurons of METH-injected mice, we examined the effect of baclofen on the induced firing rate of GABA neurons (Figure 8). We predicted that a loss of GABABR-GIRK signaling would attenuate GABABR-mediated

suppression of firing in GABA neurons. To test this, a series of current steps (20–100 pA) were injected to elicit a train of action potentials in GABA neurons (Figures 8A and 8B). In saline- and METH-injected mice, the input-output (I-O) plot shows a linear increase in firing rate with larger current injections (Figures 8B and 8D). As expected, baclofen (100 μM) significantly suppressed firing in GABA neurons of saline-injected mice, decreasing the slope of the I-O curve (Figures 8A and 8B). By contrast, a saturating dose of baclofen (100 μM) did not significantly change the I-O curve in METH-injected mice (Figures 8B and 8C). These results demonstrate that a loss of GABABR-GIRK currents in GABA neurons removes an important “brake” on GABA neuron firing in the VTA. Drug-evoked synaptic plasticity can cause persistent modifications of neural circuits that eventually lead to addiction.

Another possibility is that the smell of PA14 is recognized throu

Another possibility is that the smell of PA14 is recognized through an as yet unidentified odorant molecule that is unique to it. Next, we asked which neurons operate downstream of AWB and AWC to display naive and learned olfactory preferences. Serial-section electron micrography has revealed the complete wiring diagram of the C. elegans nervous system ( Chen et al., 2006 and White et al., 1986). Potential functional circuits can be identified as groups MK-2206 ic50 of neurons that are heavily interconnected by large numbers of synapses. This straightforward method of counting synapses allowed previous investigators, for example,

to map pathways that regulate the ability of crawling worms to generate spontaneous omega turns and reversals ( Gray et al., selleck chemicals llc 2005). We applied this method to identify integrated circuits downstream of AWB and AWC based on strong chemical synaptic connections ( Table S1 and Supplemental Experimental Procedures). This analysis uncovered a multilayered

neural network composed of sensory neurons (AWB, AWC, and ADF), interneurons (AIB, AIY, AIZ, RIA, and RIB), and motor neurons (RIM, RIV, RMD, SAA, and SMD) ( Figure 2C; Table S2). Most neurons in this candidate network represent pairs of bilaterally symmetric neurons except that SMD and SAA are groups of four neurons and RMD are six neurons. This network downstream of the AWB and AWC olfactory sensory neurons overlaps partly with a previously mapped network that regulates the frequency of reversals and omega turns of crawling worms ( Gray et al., CYTH4 2005 and Tsalik and Hobert, 2003). For this study, the network downstream of AWB and AWC provides candidate pathways for understanding how olfactory preference and plasticity are generated in the C. elegans nervous system. To characterize how the neuronal function of the olfactory network in Figure 2C allows animals to display different olfactory preferences before and after learning, we performed a systematic laser ablation analysis of

each neuronal type in the network. We conducted laser ablations with a femtosecond laser microbeam (Chung et al., 2006) on L2 larvae and cultivated the operated animals under standard conditions until the adult stage. At this point, we transferred half of the operated animals onto a fresh lawn of OP50 as naive controls and the other half onto a fresh lawn of PA14 to induce aversive olfactory learning (Figure 1A). After 6 hours, we measured turning frequency of these naive and trained animals toward alternating air streams odorized with OP50 or PA14 (Figures 5G and 6G), which allowed us to analyze choice indexes to quantify olfactory preference (Figures 1B and 1C). We quantified effects of neuronal ablation by comparing the choice indexes of naive and trained ablated animals with those of matched mock controls.

However, unlike RA-LTMRs that associate with guard and awl/auchen

However, unlike RA-LTMRs that associate with guard and awl/auchene follicles of the mouse, Aδ-LTMR lanceolate endings are found around awl/auchene and zigzag, but not guard hair follicles (Li et al., 2011) (Figure 1B). C-LTMRs. Though C fibers are often associated with painful stimuli, mechanoreceptors with conduction velocities within the C fiber range were described in the cat as early as 1939

by Ingve Zotterman (1939) and suggested to be associated with “tickling” sensations. Subsequent research on C-LTMRs indeed established that not all cutaneous sensory receptors http://www.selleckchem.com/products/KU-55933.html with afferent C fibers are concerned with relaying noxious information (Douglas and Ritchie, 1957, Iggo, 1960 and Iggo and Kornhuber, 1977). In addition, since sensory C fibers are three to four times more numerous than A fibers, C-LTMRs far outnumber the myelinated fibers innervating skin (Li et al., 2011). Like Aδ-LTMRs, C-LTMRs are exquisitely sensitive to skin indentation but are maximally activated by stimuli that move slowly across their receptive

field and are thus suggested to be “caress click here detectors.” The C-LTMR physiological profile is unique among hairy skin LTMRs. Most notably, they exhibit an intermediately adapting property, with a modest sustained discharge during a maintained stimulus (Table 1). Unlike other hairy skin LTMRs, C-LTMRs also show a high incidence of after-discharge, even several seconds after the stimulus is removed. The shape of their action potentials is characteristic of C fibers, with broad waveforms displaying a prominent hump on the falling phase. As with Aδ-LTMRs, C-LTMRs are sensitive to rapid cooling, but not warming, of the skin; however, it is unclear whether the temperatures to which these receptors respond to are physiologically relevant

for the behaving animal. One of the most striking features of C-LTMR responses is that they are only found in hairy skin. Though less common in nonhuman primate skin, C-LTMRs are present in human hairy skin and are speculated to play a role in mediating “emotional touch” (Kumazawa and many Perl, 1977, Löken et al., 2009, McGlone et al., 2007 and Vallbo et al., 1993). Indeed, in humans lacking large myelinated fibers, activation of C-LTMRs is correlated with a sensation of pleasantness often associated with activation of the insular but not the somatosensory cortex (Björnsdotter et al., 2009 and Olausson et al., 2002). The peripheral and central anatomy of C-LTMRs was largely unknown until recent studies in the mouse postulated that they may have several anatomical forms in hairy skin. Postrecording intracellular labeling of C-LTMRs identified in ex vivo skin nerve recordings revealed that C-LTMRs express tyrosine hydroxylase (TH). By utilizing a CreER knocked into the TH locus, Li et al.

However, ischemic strokes are often

associated with many

However, ischemic strokes are often

associated with many of the vascular pathologies described below, which also contribute to the total vascular burden. By far, the most prevalent vascular lesions associated with VCI are related SB203580 to alterations in small vessels in the hemispheric white matter (Jellinger, 2013). These microvascular alterations result in different neuropathological lesions, which can occur in isolation but, more typically, coexist in the same brain (Table 1). Confluent white matter lesions, the imaging correlate of which is termed leukoaraiosis (Figure 3), and lacunes, small (<1.5 cm) white matter infarcts typically in the basal ganglia, are common occurrence in VCI and are strongly associated with cardiovascular risk factors, especially hypertension, diabetes, hyperlipidemia, and smoking (Gorelick et al., 2011, Wardlaw et al., 2013a and Wardlaw

et al., 2013b). The vascular pathologies underlying these lesions consist of atherosclerotic plaques affecting small cerebral vessels, deposition of a hyaline substance in the vascular wall (lipohyalinosis), fibrotic changes in the vessel wall resulting in stiffening and microvascular distortion (arteriolosclerosis), and total loss of integrity of the vascular wall (fibrinoid necrosis) Dabrafenib (Figure 5) (Thal et al., 2012). Arterioles become tortuous, have thickened basement membranes, and are surrounded STK38 by enlarged perivascular spaces (Brown and Thore, 2011). Capillaries are reduced in number and “string vessels,” nonfunctional capillaries that have lost endothelial cells and have only a basement membrane, are observed (Brown and Thore, 2011). Collagen deposits are observed in venules (venous collagenosis) (Black et al., 2009 and Brown and Thore, 2011). The white matter damage resulting from these lesions consists of vacuolation, demyelination, axonal loss, and lacunar infarcts.

The white matter lesions generally correspond to hyperintensities observed on MRI, which, however, can also reflect other pathological substrates (Gouw et al., 2011). The white matter lesions evolve over time by expansion of existing lesions, rather than formation of new foci (Maillard et al., 2012), resembling the patterns of progression of amyloid angiopathy (Alonzo et al., 1998 and Robbins et al., 2006). The expansion of the white matter lesions correlates with the evolution of the cognitive impairment (Maillard et al., 2012), new lacunes causing a steeper decline, especially in motor speed and executive functions (Jokinen et al., 2011). White matter lesions and lacunar infarcts are also present in uncommon genetic conditions resulting in VCI and vascular dementia (Federico et al., 2012 and Schmidt et al., 2012). The better studied of these, CADASIL, is associated with extensive leukoaraiosis and lacunar infarcts (Chabriat et al., 2009).

The two factors were pursuit (on/off) and 2D planar motion (on/of

The two factors were pursuit (on/off) and 2D planar motion (on/off) (Figure 1A). During 2D planar BKM120 cost motion the entire dot field moved sinusoidally along the vertical and horizontal axes with three or four cycles per trial (randomly assigned, respectively) and with random initial

phases and directions, resulting in smooth sinusoidal 2D planar trajectories of 5 visual degrees in diameter (Figure 1B). During pursuit the otherwise central fixation disc (that contained the task, see below) moved along the same trajectory (also 5° in diameter). When both pursuit and planar motion were “on,” the fixation task moved locked together with the dots, resulting in zero planar retinal motion. The mean (median) dot/pursuit speed was 3.80 (3.80) °/s, and the maximal eccentricity of the fixation disc reached 2.5°. A GLM analysis

of this 2 × 2 factorial design allowed us to separate cortical responses related to the main factors of (1) eye movements (pursuit), (2) objective (2D planar) motion, and their interaction (3) retinal motion. Both (2) 2D planar motion and (3) retinal motion were balanced for conditions with and without pursuit (see Figure 1), and were thus not confounded by effects related to pursuit (such as peripheral motion http://www.selleckchem.com/products/CAL-101.html induced by the screen edges, or potentially less accurate fixation during pursuit). Experiment 2 was identical to experiment 1 but used 1D (horizontal only) trajectories with four cycles per trial (see Figures 1A and 1C), and the speed of the motion trajectory was changed from a sine function to abs(sin(t))(1/3) in order to achieve a more linear velocity profile. The mean (median) dot/pursuit speed was 3.30 (2.3)

°/s. During this experiment, eye movements were recorded inside the scanner. Experiment 3 was identical to experiment 2, but expansion/contraction flow was added to all stimuli, as illustrated in Figure 6A. The flow alternated between contraction and expansion with a period of four cycles per trial (same velocity profile as planar motion), and with matched mean (median) dot speeds for Resminostat pure 3D flow of 3.2 (2.3) °/s [in condition 3D(−/−)]. In each trial, starting directions for left/right and forward/backward motion were determined randomly and independently. The flow simulated forward-backward motion of a 3D dot cloud with a visibility of 0.4–2.40 m distance to the observer, and a simulated maximal (mean) velocity of 0.67 (0.55) m/s. The focus of expansion (FOE) was locked to objective planar motion, i.e., was centered and stationary in conditions 3D(+/−) and 3D(−/−), and moved in 3D(−/+) and 3D(+/+). Eye movements were recorded inside the scanner during this experiment. Experiment 4 was a replication of experiment 2, with the following four additional conditions: (−/+50%), (−/+150%), (+/+50%), and (+/+150%) (see Figure 7A). The percentages refer to the objective motion velocities that were either 50% slower or 50% faster than that of the original (−/+) and (+/+) conditions.

Our results satisfy all three of these criteria, so interpreting

Our results satisfy all three of these criteria, so interpreting the activity in the FOF as “movement preparation” is, at least, consistent with prior work. There are several possible interpretations as to what component(s) of response preparation FOF neurons

might encode: do they represent a motor plan? A memory of the identity of the motor plan? Attention? Intention? (Bisley and Goldberg, 2010, Glimcher, 2003, Goldman-Rakic et al., 1992, Schall, 2001, Thompson et al., 2005 and Gold Cabozantinib purchase and Shadlen, 2001). Our data do not discriminate between these possibilities. Nevertheless, we conclude that, as in the primate, there exists in the rat frontal cortex a structure that is involved in the preparation and/or planning of orienting responses. An area with such a role may be conserved across multiple species, including birds (Knudsen et al., 1995). Since FOF delay period firing rates are better correlated with the upcoming motor act than with the initial sensory cue (Figure 4), our data do indicate that FOF neurons are not likely to encode a memory of the auditory stimulus itself. Furthermore, in memory trials, some form of memory is required immediately after the end of the auditory instruction stimulus. We did not observe

a short-latency sensory response in the FOF, but instead observed a slow and gradual development of choice-dependent activity during the delay period. This suggests that FOF neurons do not support the early memory the task requires. The FOF is strongly interconnected with the posterior PF-06463922 molecular weight Vasopressin Receptor parietal cortex (PPC) (Reep and Corwin, 2009 and Nakamura, 1999) and with the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC, Condé et al., 1995). We suggest both of these areas as candidates for supporting the early memory aspects of

the task, perhaps even including the transformation from a continuous auditory signal (click-rate) to a binary choice (plan-left/plan-right). Based on data from an orienting task driven by olfactory stimuli, Felsen and Mainen (2008) recently proposed that the superior colliculus (SC) may play a broad role in sensory-guided orienting. Projections to the SC from the FOF (Leonard, 1969, Künzle et al., 1976 and Reep et al., 1987), together with our current data, suggest that the FOF may be an important contributor to orienting-related activity in the SC. As in the primate, orienting behavior in the rodent is likely to be subserved by a network of interacting brain areas. The relative roles and mutual interactions between the FOF, PPC, mPFC, and SC (and possibly other areas, including the basal ganglia) during orienting behaviors in the rat remain to be elucidated. We focused our analyses here on the response-selective delay period activity of FOF neurons.

Curiously, the kinetics of KARs imposed by Neto protein is remark

Curiously, the kinetics of KARs imposed by Neto protein is remarkably similar to those of NMDARs (see Figure 1). The difference between NMDAR activation kinetics and that of the faster AMPARs provides adequate timing for activation, since Pfizer Licensed Compound Library in vitro the Mg2+ blockade implies that NMDAR activation would not be operative until sufficient membrane depolarization is attained. In contrast, the functional significance of the slower kinetics of KARs is starting to be illustrated by examples that provide comprehensive roles

for such a prolonged current in synaptic integration (Frerking and Ohliger-Frerking, 2002, Goldin et al., 2007, Sachidhanandam et al., 2009 and Pinheiro et al., 2013; see Figure 1). Another striking action of Neto1 and Neto2 is that association with these proteins greatly reduces inward rectification of KAR-mediated currents without modifying Ca2+ permeability (Fisher and Mott, 2012). It seems that three positive charges (RKK) in the C-terminal of Neto proteins preclude internal polyamine blockade of KAR channel. This effect is reminiscent of stargazin in AMPARs (Soto et al., 2007). However, the functional Alectinib ic50 implication of this action remains to be defined. Apart from the clear effect of Netos on KAR channel gating and on current amplitudes (see Copits and Swanson, 2012), it remains unclear whether Neto proteins are involved

in KAR targeting to the synapse, although there is weak evidence indicating that this may be possible. Cultured hippocampal neurons express native KARs, but these are not targeted to synapses (Lerma et al., 1997). However, a small proportion of ESPCs may be mediated by KARs when such cells are transfected with Neto2 and GluK1, indicating that exogenous Neto2 may target a small proportion of exogenous GluK1 to synapses (Copits et al., 2011). Similar effects were observed in cerebellar granule cells and with GluK2 (Zhang et al., 2009). However, although GluK2 association with PSD95 is reduced in Neto2 null mice 17-DMAG (Alvespimycin) HCl (Tang et al., 2012), the lack of Neto2 expression does

not prevent the presence of endogenous GluK1 or GluK2 in synaptic contacts, despite the fact that synaptic KARs are normally associated with Netos in hippocampal slices. Indeed, KAR-mediated EPSCs in brain slices display distinct kinetics in Neto-deficient animals and EPSCKARs are present in mice even deficient for the two Neto proteins, yet with fast kinetics, consistent with the idea that Netos are not key elements in the targeting of KARs to the synapse (Tang et al., 2011). From these data, it is clear that Netos exert an important influence on KARs, which may vary depending on the subunit combination. However, Neto proteins are not specific to KARs. Indeed, Neto1 was initially identified as a NMDAR interactor.

We find that LRRTM1 and LRRTM2 DKD in vivo blocks LTP in neonatal

We find that LRRTM1 and LRRTM2 DKD in vivo blocks LTP in neonatal CA1 pyramidal neurons, a deficit that is rescued by wild-type LRRTM2. Further replacement experiments revealed that the extracellular, but not intracellular, domain of LRRTM2 is required for LTP. LTP was not rescued by expression of a mutant LRRTM2 reported to impair binding to Nrxs Tariquidar in vitro (Siddiqui et al., 2010), although whether this mutant quantitatively reaches the surface to the same degree as wild-type LRRTM2 is unknown. Importantly, LRRTM1 and LRRTM2 DKD in adult CA1 pyramidal neurons in vivo

also strongly impaired LTP. These results demonstrate that the block of LTP by LRRTM1 and LRRTM2 DKD is not due to some unknown effect on synapse maturation but rather to a critical role of LRRTMs in LTP at mature synapses. A cell culture model of LTP provided further insight into the mechanisms by which LRRTMs may function in LTP. LRRTM1 and LRRTM2 Compound Library DKD blocked this model of LTP and surprisingly increased the net surface expression of AMPARs under basal conditions. Immunocytochemical and electrophysiological assays revealed that DKD caused an increase in surface expression of extrasynaptic AMPARs while decreasing synaptic AMPARs. Furthermore, the DKD did not affect the initial increase in surface and synaptic AMPAR expression 10 min after

cLTP induction yet caused a decrease in net AMPAR surface expression when measured 20 min after cLTP. All of the effects of the DKD in cultured neurons were reversed by wild-type LRRTM2, suggesting that the phenotypes were Idoxuridine not due to off-target effects. The results in cultured neurons are consistent with the decrease in AMPAR-mediated synaptic transmission caused by LRRTM DKD in vivo in neonatal hippocampus (de Wit et al., 2009 and Soler-Llavina et al., 2011) as well as the time course of the block of LTP in acute slices. They support the hypothesis that LRRTMs are required for maintaining a normal complement of synaptic AMPARs to support basal synaptic transmission but not for the AMPAR exocytosis that occurs after LTP induction. However, in adult CA1 pyramidal neurons, LRRTM1 and LRRTM2 DKD

did not have a detectable effect on basal AMPAR-mediated synaptic transmission (Soler-Llavina et al., 2011). A simple hypothesis to explain all of these results is that in young, developing synapses LRRTMs serve two functions. They help maintain a normal complement of synaptic AMPARs for basal synaptic transmission and, after LTP induction, they contribute to the scaffolding or “slot” complex that stabilizes the newly delivered AMPARs (Malinow and Malenka, 2002 and Opazo and Choquet, 2011). In their absence after LTP induction, AMPARs transiently diffuse into but cannot be maintained within the PSD; they escape to sites at which endocytosis occurs, a process that may have been accelerated by the LTP induction protocol.

Indeed, several molecules and signaling pathways

recently

Indeed, several molecules and signaling pathways

recently shown to be involved in visual map development were initially identified through differential screens for genes regulated by neuronal activity (e.g., Shatz, 2009). The results described here show that even rather subtle genetic manipulations that only alter patterns of spontaneous activity without changing the levels of activity can have a profound impact on brain development. This may have significant implications for diseases check details of multigenetic origin, such as schizophrenia and autism, in which brain wiring may be negatively affected not because of direct effects of genes on neural circuits or synaptic function, but because of indirect effects on patterns of spontaneous or evoked activity during neural circuit development. β2-nAChR subunit knockout β2(KO) and transgenic β2(TG) mice with retina-specific expression of β2-nAChRs were generated as described (King et al., 2003). Wild-type (WT) mice (C57BL/6J) were

obtained from Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME). Doxycycline administration was provided through the mothers of experimental Afatinib clinical trial mice via water containing doxycycline (1mg/ml) from E0 to P8. Animals were treated in compliance with the Yale IACUC, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, and Institution guidelines. Focal DiI injections (2.3 nl) for measurements of retinotopy were performed,

imaged and quantified blind to genotype as described (Chandrasekaran et al., 2005). Injections were localized along the perimeter of the retina, using as a reference the insertion points of the four major eye muscles (Plas et al., 2005). Retinal injection size, quantified by measuring the area of fluorescent signal in the retina above one-half of the maximum fluorescent signal after background subtraction, showed no difference across all genotypes and injection locations, and there was no relationship between TZ area and retinal injection area (Figure S7; McLaughlin et al., 2003). Measurements of eye-specific segregation were performed with whole eye injections (1 μl into the vitreous) of Alexa Fluor 488-conjugated cholera toxin (left eye) and Alexa Fluor 594 (right eye) at P6, then returned to their mother for 24–48 hr crotamiton to allow transport of tracer from the retina to the SC and dLGN. CPT-cAMP treated animals were injected daily with 500 nl of saline or CPT-cAMP (5 mM) into both eyes from P2 to P6, then received whole eye injections of Alexa dye at P7. Eye-specific segregation in the SC was quantified by measuring the fraction of fluorescence signal labeled from the ipsilateral eye in the SGS layer, and also by measuring the overlap (in % of pixels) of ipsilateral eye fluorescence signal with contralateral eye fluorescence signal in the SGS layer.

Importantly, these dysfunctions are largely immune to current ant

Importantly, these dysfunctions are largely immune to current antipsychotic treatments and, as a result, constitute a major determinant

for psychosocial functioning and outcome (Green, 1996). The identification of the causes of dysfunctional cognition is, therefore, a prerequisite for the developmental of novel and more effective interventions. The search for the underlying pathophysiological processes has thus far focused on anatomical Antidiabetic Compound Library and functional abnormalities in circumscribed brain regions. This approach has yielded a large body of evidence implicating various brain areas in cognitive deficits, but the precise circuits and mechanisms underlying these dysfunctions have remained elusive. An alternative approach has been the focus on the role of impaired communication between regions in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia, which most likely involves a disconnection of functional networks (Friston, 1998). This hypothesis has received support through

findings from noninvasive studies using electro- and magneto-encephalography (EEG/MEG) that demonstrate impaired amplitude and synchrony of neural oscillations at low- and high-frequency ranges in patients with schizophrenia (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2010). This is of particular relevance because a large body of evidence suggests that the functional networks underlying perception, attention, and executive processes rely on dynamic coordination selleck screening library through the phase locking of synchronized oscillations (Varela et al., 2001). Accordingly, impairments in this mechanism could lead to a transient failure in the first establishment of functional interactions between brain regions, thereby affecting the associated cognitive processes. In this issue of Neuron, Parnaudeau et al. (2013) investigated the hypothesis that thalamocortical synchronization, in this case, between frontal

brain regions and the mediodorsal (MD) thalamus, might play an important role in WM and that disturbed synchrony in this circuit might be responsible for WM impairments in schizophrenia. Thalamic functions have recently received renewed interest in systems neuroscience because of their crucial role in gating communication between cortical areas through the synchronization of neuronal responses ( Saalmann et al., 2012). Because anatomical and functional abnormalities have been repeatedly demonstrated in the thalamus of patients with schizophrenia ( Ronenwett and Csernansky, 2010), abnormal synchronization in thalamocortical pathways could represent an intriguing pathophysiological mechanism for cognitive impairments. To test this hypothesis, the authors employed a novel pharmacogenetic approach (designer receptors exclusively activated by designer drugs [DREADD]) (Armbruster et al.